echemi logo
Product
  • Product
  • Supplier
  • Inquiry
    Home > Active Ingredient News > Study of Nervous System > eLife—Wang Liping group discovered neural computing of causal inference in the frontal parietal loop of macaques

    eLife—Wang Liping group discovered neural computing of causal inference in the frontal parietal loop of macaques

    • Last Update: 2023-01-06
    • Source: Internet
    • Author: User
    Search more information of high quality chemicals, good prices and reliable suppliers, visit www.echemi.com

    Written by Qi Guangyao - Wang Sizhen, Fang Yiyi edited by Xia Ye


    In daily life, we often receive a variety of sensory information
    .
    Properly integrating multisensory information can help us respond faster and more accurately
    [1].

    Integrate or separate? This is the
    binding problem of information [2].

    However, this process relies on our accurate inference
    of the source of the information.
    For example, when you cross the street, you will receive visual and sound information from the vehicle, both of which can reflect the
    vehicle's location information to some extent.
    When you infer that these two pieces of information come from the same source (the same vehicle), you integrate the two together for localization, otherwise you separate the information to locate
    .
    This process of inferring hidden causal structures based on observations is causal
    inference[3].

    Causal inference is influenced by two factors: one is the influence of top-down prior information, such as your past experience, the street you are in; On the other hand, there are bottom-up effects, such as the spatial or temporal difference between the two sensory information
    [4].

    Previous studies have shown that people integrate these two factors through Bayesian rules to obtain
    a posterior probability of common source (Pcom) for a causal structure That is, the probabilistic inference of the causal structure [3, 5].

    However, although previous studies have found that the computational process of causal inference obeys the bottom-up cortical hierarchy [2], there are still the following limitations: on the one hand, previous studies have mainly focused on human brain imaging, and lack the understanding of neural circuits at the level of single neurons; On the other hand, previous studies have focused on the neural relevance of behavioral representations, while how the brain represents hidden causal structures and how prior probability of common source (Pprior) and sensory representations are dynamically described remain unknown
    .


    On October 24, 2022, the Wang Liping Research Group of the Center for Excellence in Brain Science and Intelligent Technology (Institute of Neuroscience) of the Chinese Academy of Sciences published an online title in the eLife journal The research paper "Neural dynamics of causal inference in the macaque frontoparietal circuit" is the first to describe neural characterization and computational mechanisms
    at the neural circuit level of causal inference.
    PhD student Qi Guangyao and postdoctoral fellow Fang Wen are the co-first authors of the paper, and researcher Wang Liping and postdoctoral fellow Fang Wen are co-corresponding authors
    .
    This study uses a virtual reality system to perform electrophysiological recordings while awake macaques complete a causal inference task, and combines Bayesian modeling analysis and machine learning methods to first extend the original static causal inference model on the computational model, revealing how prior knowledge and sensory information in the causal inference model are updated with changes in the environment.
    Secondly, it is found that (
    i) prefrontal motor cortex neurons characterize and update causal structures at the loop realization level to solve the binding problem of multimodal information; (ii) To maintain their consistency with the inferred environment, parietal cortical neurons receive premotor cortex feedback and update the characterization of sensory information to support the plasticity
    of the body's sense of self-awareness.
    The study discovered a new computational system for causal inference
    .



    In order to study the neural circuit characterization and computer system of causal inference, the researchers first designed a causal inference task based on a virtual reality system that can change the spatial position difference between visual and proprioceptive sensation (Figure 1A) [6].

    In this task, macaques need to
    "report" the position of their arms based on the visual information and proprioception they receive (Figure 1B).

    This task condition is called
    a Visual-proprioceptive conflict (VPC).
    condition
    。 In this task, macaques infer a posterior probability (causal inference) that visual and proprioceptive information comes from the same source, and use this as a weight to estimate the position of the final arm
    [6].

    In addition to
    VPC conditions, macaques require pure proprioception only (P) conditions and visual-proprioceptive congruence conditions Visual-proprioceptive congruent (VP) condition
    。 Here
    , P and VP serve as reference baselines
    for complete separation and full integration, respectively.


    Figure 1: Results of behavioral tasks, dynamic causal inference models, and proprioceptive bias

    (Source: Qi, et al.
    , eLife, 2022).


    In order to study the dynamic update of macaque behavior in the process of causal inference, combined with this experimental paradigm, the researchers proposed a dynamic causal inference model
    .
    This model adds an updated part of prior knowledge and proprioception to the previous Bayesian causal inference model
    (Figure 1C).

    The model assumes the following:


    First, macaques combine sensory information (visual and proprioception) and prior information in a Bayesian manner to judge arm position (Figure 1C).

    Therefore, the behavioral output of macaques, that is
    , the distance of the arm subjectively reported by different visual differences (proprioceptive shift, Drift), will exhibit typical causal inference characteristics
    .
    For smaller
    disparity, Pcom should be added (consolidated); When Disparity gets bigger, Pcom should decrease (detach).


    Second, macaques at the single trial level, the P of the current trial is subject to the environment (the P of the previous trial com and adjust the effect of current causal inference of prior information by updating P prior in the current state (Figure 1C1).


    Third, in order to maintain consistency with a priori beliefs about causal structures, the early processing of sensory information is also influenced by the environment
    .
    When proprioceptive and visual information collide (e.
    g.
    , VPC tasks), the brain, in addition to reducing the prior belief that this information comes from a source (Pprior), also reduces the precision of sensory representations, thus balancing the credibility of current sensory signals and prior beliefs (figure).
    1C3


    To test these hypotheses, the researchers used dynamic causal inference models to evaluate monkey behavior and studied the implementation
    of neural circuits in multiple brain regions.


    Consistent with the model's assumptions, the behavioral results of macaques show that: (i) the proprioceptive bias of macaques exhibits a nonlinear dependence on differences between proprioceptive and visual inputs, i.
    e.
    , when Disparity is smaller, macaques tend to integrate the two types of information, while when When Disparity is large, macaques tend to separate the two types of information (Figure 1D).

    The causal inference model explains this well
    (Figure 1D, 2B), i.
    e.
    when
    Disparity is small, Pcom Larger, macaques tend to integrate two types of information; While when Disparity is larger and Pcom is smaller, macaques tend to separate the two types of information
    .
    (ii) P priori is combined with sensory input and updated on a trial-by-trial basis based on historical information (Pcom).
    (Figure 2A).

    (
    iii) To maintain consistency in causal inference, proprioceptive accuracy is updated during causal inference as Pcom changes (Figure 2C).

    The above results validate the model's hypothesis at the behavioral level that macaques adapt to environmental changes by integrating prior information and sensory inputs to infer hidden causal structures, while dynamically updating P prior and sensory representations.

    This laid a theoretical foundation
    for the subsequent study of the neural circuit mechanism of causal inference.


    Figure 2: The causal inference model predicts the dynamic update of monkey behavior

    (Source: Qi, et al.
    , eLife, 2022).


    Next, in order to study the neural mechanism of causal inference, the researchers recorded the premotor cortex (PMC) and parietal cortex (Parietal area 5), which are closely related to multisensory perception and body representation, during the macaque performing causal inference tasks and area 7) neuronal activity [7, 8].

    Through information encoding
    and decoding analysis, the researchers analyzed the neural circuit mechanism
    of dynamic computation of causal inference.
    At the single-neuron level, the researchers found that task-related information was widely present in the frontopietal circuit
    .
    1) For sensory signals, parietal cortical neurons encode more proprioceptive arm position information, while PMC neurons encode more arm position information
    after integration.
    2) In addition to PMCs, a certain proportion (>5%) of the coding Pcom is also present in the parietal cortex of neurons (causal inference neurons).

    3) At the population neuron level, by combining machine learning algorithms to decode and analyze the task components and latent variable - Pcom, the researchers found that the update of the hidden causal structure only appeared in PMCs , and its characteristic causal inference predates the parietal cortex, and then transmits signals to the parietal cortex through the communication subspace (Figures 3A and B); 4) The parietal cortex receives Pcom information from PMCs and adapts to the environment by updating the encoding of proprioception (Figures 3B and C).

    These results validate the hypotheses and behavioral observations of dynamic causal inference models from the perspective of neural activity
    .


    Figure 3: Neural implementation of dynamic causal inference

    (Source: Qi, et al.
    , eLife, 2022).


    Conclusions, discussions, inspirations and outlooks, the behavioral and multicephalal electrophysiological data of this study reveal for the first time the neural representation and computational system
    of causal inference at single-neuron resolution in the frontopietal circuit.
    The study extends previous studies on the premotor cortex to parietal cortical neurons
    [6]; The previous static causal inference model is extended [3, 5], and a new dynamic causal inference model is proposed.
    It was found that the posterior probability of the same source was a trial-by-trial dynamic update of neuronal activity in the premotor cortex, while the update of the sensory representation of the input occurred in the neuronal activity
    of the parietal cortex on a larger time scale.
    The study provides a theoretical and neural circuit basis
    for answering scientific questions such as how the brain solves the problem of multisensory information binding and body representation updating in the body's self-awareness.


    Future studies can be carried out in the following directions: First, the results of this study complement
    previous findings in mirror neuron systems in the premotor cortex and parietal cortex in humans and monkeys.
    Future research will need to examine how the two systems work together to identify self and foreign agents

    .
    Second, clinically, pathological disorders inferred of sensory origin lead to somatic hallucinations
    [9].

    Schizophrenic patients with surrogate delusion disorder show impairment
    in updating their internal causal structures.
    They show deficiencies in detecting the source of their thoughts and actions, so they mistakenly attribute them to external agents
    [10].

    Future studies can use this research paradigm to further explore in schizophrenia patients and use causal inference models to find biomarkers for diagnosing schizophrenia patients
    .


    Original link: https://elifesciences.
    org/articles/76145


    The research was mainly completed by doctoral student Qi Guangyao and postdoctoral fellow Fang Wen under the guidance of researcher Wang Liping, and doctoral student Li Shenghao and research assistant Li Junru also actively participated
    .
    The work was funded
    by the Ministry of Science and Technology, Lingang Laboratory, the Chinese Academy of Sciences, and the Shanghai Municipal Science and Technology Commission.


    First author: Qi Guangyao (second from left); First author and corresponding author: Fang Wen (third from left); Authors: Li Shenghao (first from left), Li Junru (fourth from left).

    (Photo courtesy of Wang Liping Laboratory)


    First author: Qi Guangyao (right); Corresponding author: Wang Liping (left).
     

    (Photo courtesy of Wang Liping Laboratory)



    Welcome to scan the code to join Logical Neuroscience Literature Study 3

    Group Note Format: Name--Field of Research-Degree/Title/Title/PositionSelected Previous Articles



    [1] Ageing Res Rev Review—New insights between gut microbiome ecological disorders and Parkinson's disease: pathogenicity and clinical relevance

    [2] Sci Adv-Tai Yanlong's research group proposed an artificial remote sensing tactile device with three-dimensional depth perception

    【3】 Cell Res—Zheng Hui/Xu Xingshun team reveals the mechanism of depression-induced antiviral immune dysfunction

    【4】 The Mol Neurobiol—Xu Kaibiao/Gao Yibo team discovered the underlying pathological mechanism of new-onset refractory status epilepticus caused by different causes

    [5] Mol Psychiatry—Pang Zhiping/Chen Chao/Nobel laureate Thomas Südhof's team revealed a new mechanism of sexual function synaptic acquired by autism risk mutations

    【6】 The PLOS Biol-Lu team found that the sleep-wake cycle dynamically regulates hippocampal inhibitory synaptic plasticity

    【7】 J Neuroinflammation—Zhuo Yehong/Su Wenru's team revealed that iron death may be a new mechanism and therapeutic target for retinal ischemia-reperfusion

    [8] PNAS-Zhong Yi's group revealed that the amnesia mechanism of Rac1-dependence is the neural basis for emotional states to affect memory expression

    【9】 Cell Metab Review—Xu Cao's team reviews the regulation of bone homeostasis and bone pain by the intraosseous sensory system

    【10】 The NAN-Yuan Linhong group revealed that DHA intervention had different effects on brain lipid levels, fatty acid transporter expression and Aβ metabolism in ApoE-/- and C57 WT mice

    Recommended high-quality scientific research training courses [1] The 10th NIR Training Camp (online: 2022.
    11.
    30~12.
    20) [2] The 9th EEG Data Analysis Flight (Training Camp: 2022.
    11.
    23-12.
    24)
    Welcome to join "Logical Neuroscience" [1] " Logical Neuroscience " Recruitment Editor/Operation Position ( Online Office [2] Talent Recruitment - "Logical Neuroscience" Recruitment Article Interpretation/Writing Position ( Online Part-time, Online Office) References (swipe up and down to read).



    1.
      Stein, B.
    E.
    and T.
    R.
    Stanford, Multisensory integration: current issues from the perspective of the single neuron.
     Nat Rev Neurosci, 2008.
     9(4): p.
    255-66.

    2.
    Noppeney, U.
    , Perceptual Inference, Learning, and Attention in a Multisensory World.
     Annu Rev Neurosci, 2021.
     44: p.
    449-473.

    3.
    Kording, K.
    P.
    , et al.
    , Causal inference in multisensory perception.
     PLoS One, 2007.
     2(9): p.
    e943.

    4.
    Shams, L.
    and U.
    R.
    Beierholm, Causal inference in perception.
     Trends Cogn Sci, 2010.
     14(9): p.
    425-32.

    5.
    Sato, Y.
    , T.
    Toyoizumi, and K.
    Aihara, Bayesian inference explains perception of unity and ventriloquism aftereffect: identification of common sources of audiovisual stimuli.
     Neural computation, 2007.
     19(12): p.
    3335-3355.

    6.
     Fang, W.
    , et al.
    , Statistical inference of body representation in the macaque brain.
     Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 2019.
     116(40): p.
    20151-20157.

    7.
    Blanke, O.
    , M.
    Slater, and A.
    Serino,
    Behavioral, Neural, and Computational Principles of Bodily Self-Consciousness.
    Neuron, 2015.
    88(1): p.
    145-66.

    8.
    Graziano, M.
    S.
    A.
    and M.
    M.
    Botvinick,
    How the brain represents the body: insights from neurophysiology and psychology.
    Common Mechanisms in Perception and Action, 2002.
    19: p.
    136-157.

    9.
    Keromnes, G.
    , et al.
    , Exploring Self-Consciousness From Self- and Other-Image Recognition in the Mirror: Concepts and Evaluation.
     Front Psychol, 2019.
     10: p.
    719.

    10.
    Haggard, P.
    , Sense of agency in the human brain.
     Nat Rev Neurosci, 2017.
     18(4): p.
    196-207.


    End of article

    This article is an English version of an article which is originally in the Chinese language on echemi.com and is provided for information purposes only. This website makes no representation or warranty of any kind, either expressed or implied, as to the accuracy, completeness ownership or reliability of the article or any translations thereof. If you have any concerns or complaints relating to the article, please send an email, providing a detailed description of the concern or complaint, to service@echemi.com. A staff member will contact you within 5 working days. Once verified, infringing content will be removed immediately.

    Contact Us

    The source of this page with content of products and services is from Internet, which doesn't represent ECHEMI's opinion. If you have any queries, please write to service@echemi.com. It will be replied within 5 days.

    Moreover, if you find any instances of plagiarism from the page, please send email to service@echemi.com with relevant evidence.